自左至右,分别为鸸鹋鷸鴕西仓鸮原鸽的脑,并标明了视觉处理区域

鸟脑是鸟类神经系统的中心,鸟类的庞大而复杂,负责将环境中接收到的信息进行处理整合和协调,并决定身体的其他部分做出相应的反应。与所有脊索动物一样,鸟类大脑包含在头部颅骨内。

The bird brain is divided into a number of sections, each with a different function. The cerebrum or telencephalon is divided into two hemispheres, and controls higher functions. The telencephalon is dominated by a large pallium, which corresponds to the mammalian cerebral cortex and is responsible for the cognitive functions of birds. The pallium is made up of several major structures: the hyperpallium, a dorsal bulge of the pallium found only in birds, as well as the nidopallium, mesopallium, and archipallium. The bird telencephalon nuclear structure, wherein neurons are distributed in three-dimensionally arranged clusters, with no large-scale separation of white matter and grey matter, though there exist layer-like and column-like connections. Structures in the pallium are associated with perception, learning, and cognition. Beneath the pallium are the two components of the subpallium, the striatum and pallidum. The subpallium connects different parts of the telencephalon and plays major roles in a number of critical behaviours. To the rear of the telencephalon are the thalamus, midbrain, and cerebellum. The hindbrain connects the rest of the brain to the spinal cord.

The size and structure of the avian brain enables prominent behaviours of birds such as flight and vocalization. Dedicated structures and pathways integrate the auditory and visual senses, strong in most species of birds, as well as the typically weaker olfactory and tactile senses. Social behaviour, widespread among birds, depends on the organisation and functions of the brain. Some birds exhibit strong abilities of cognition, enabled by the unique structure and physiology of the avian brain.

结构和功能

编辑

Telencephalon

编辑
 
A: A view of the brain of a pigeon within the head. B: The pigeon's visual processing pathway. C:The sensory cortex of a pigeon. D: Connectosome view of the sensory cortex.

The telencephalon, or forebrain, is generally a large structure in birds. It is made up of three primary domains: the striatal domain, the pallidal domain, and the pallial domain.[1]

Pallium

编辑

The pallial domain is divided into two main parts: the Wulst, or hyperpallium, at the dorsal surface of the pallium, and the dorso-ventricular ridge, which comprises the nidopallium, arcopallium, and archipallium. The circuitry of the avian brain is organised such that circuits for processes such as vision and cognition pass between the structures of the pallium, with connections both horizontally and vertically oriented within the pallium.[2] In the sensory areas of the pallium, neurons display a layer-like organisation into columns, with horizontal connections; hence, the part of the pallium responsible for sensory circuitry is also called the sensory cortex in birds.[3]

Within the pallium, the nidopallium caudolaterale is thought to be a centre of goal-directed action. It is the part of the pallium that has the most innervation sensitive to dopamine, and has a structure suggesting strong dopamine control. Neural activity in the nidopallium caudolaterale is correlated with rewards, rules, categories, and information held in the memory. The nidopallium caudolaterale is also proportionally much larger in birds with stronger cognitive abilities. Showing diversity in bird brains and potentially the role of the nidopallium caudolaterale is that structure's separation into multiple structures in songbirds.[4]

Lateralisation
编辑

The avian telencephalon, like its human counterpart, is organised into two distinct hemispheres. Functions such as song learning in songbirds, vision, auditory, and olfactory, and magnetic sensory systems are all lateralised, meaning that one hemisphere of the brain dominates a certain function. The left hemisphere of the brain dominates activities that demand the separation of important stimuli from distracting ones, while the right hemisphere is more easily distracted and has a broad attention. The right hemisphere also dominates fear and escape responses.[5] In zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), the left hemisphere also is dominant in learning tasks.[6] Birds can sense the orientation of magnetic fields, and use them to help navigate. The right hemisphere of the brain is dominant in the magnetic sense.[5]

Subpallial telencephalon

编辑

The subpallial tenecephalon of birds, ventral to the pallium, contains neural systems with critical functions. It can be divided into the striatal domain and the pallidal domain, which are progenitor zones from which brain structures develop. The subpallial telencephalon is also grouped into several major functional systems. These are the dorsal somatomotor basal ganglia, the ventral viscerolimbic basal ganglia, subpallial extended amygdala, basal corticopetal system, and the septum. Additionally, the preoptic area is considered part of the subpallial telencephalon. Functional systems exist in both subpallial domains.[7]

Research through the 1960s demonstrated that the basal ganglia of birds occupied only the ventromedial telencephalon and not the entire forebrain, a historically held belief. The basal ganglia include the dorsal somatomotor basal ganglia (DSBG), which possesses both a pallidal and a striatal component, and is made up of the medial striatum, lateral striatum, globus pallidus, and intrapeduncular nucleus. In songbirds, it also contains Area X, which is responsible for aspects of vocal function in songbirds. The DSBG has important functions in voluntary motor control. Pathways including the DSBG allow birds to effect movement when desired and to reject movement when undesired. The basal ganglia also include the ventral viscerolimbic basal ganglia (VVBG). The VVBG, like the DSBG, occupies portion of both the striatum and pallidum. It contains the ventral portion of the medial striatum, the nucleus accumbens, the olfactory tubercle, and the ventral pallidum. The VVBG functions as a "reward centre" as well as a facilitator of "action selection". In these roles, the VVBG is important in supporting reward-seeking behaviour and discouraging behaviour leading to negative stimulus.[8]

Area X, unique to songbirds, is critical to learning song, which is a reproduction-critical behaviour in songbirds. It has been described by scientists as analogous to the mammalian striatum, due to a similar composition to that part of the brain. However, it also contains brain cells from the pallidum. Area X is activated most strongly when songbirds are learning new songs, and decline thereafter.[9]

The extended amygdala is composed of the central extended amygdala and medial extended amygdala. The central extended amygdala is connected to behaviours involving ingestion, as well as stress, anxiety, and fear. The role and structure of the medial extended amygdala are debated by scientists. The medial extended amygdala receives inputs from the olfactory bulb, as well as the rest of the olfactory system, including the olfactory organs in the front of bird heads. It is related to sexual and social behaviours. Certain structures in the median extended amygdala have been demonstrated in birds to be sexually dimorphic. In chickens (Gallus gallus), research shows that part of the median extended amygdala plays a part in male sexual behaviour.[10]

The basal corticopetal system in birds is made up of three nuclei: the basal magnocellular nucleus, and the horizontal and vertical limbs of the nuclei of the diagonal band. The role of the basal corticopetal system is poorly known in birds, although that system is known to be correlated to memory in mammals. However, it has been shown that damage to the basal corticopetal system impairs the memory of chicks (Gallus gallus).[11]

Based on studies of a variety of passerine birds, the avian septum is divided into four main parts: the lateral septum, medial septum, septohippocampal septum, and caudocentral septum. The septum has functions related to stress, as well as responses to stimuli such as light. The preoptic area has functions related to sexual behaviour.[12]

Brain stem and cerebellum

编辑

Cerebellum

编辑

The cerebellum is a relatively conservative part of the brain, with circuitry tending to be similar across different types of vertebrates. Like in mammals, the avian cerebellum is a strongly folded structure. Bird cerebella are typically divided into ten different groups of folds called lobuli. In some bird groups, the cerebellum is expanded to accommodate different requirements. For instance, in birds that perform much manipulation with the beak, like crows, woodpeckers, and parrots, there is an expansion of the visual part of the cerebellum. In contrast, the part of the cerebellum associated with the tail is expanded.[13]

There is substantial variation in the foliation (folding) in the cerebella of birds, although all birds have at least some folding of the cerebellum. The folding of the cerebellum is also not strictly associated with the structure and size of the rest of the brain: penguins, seabirds, parrots, and crows, exhibit a similar degree of folding, despite very different brain characteristics. Likewise, owls, galliformes, and pigeons exhibit similar folding patterns.[14] In general, the folding patterns of the cerebellum in birds reflect differences of behaviour, as well as variations in skull shape constraining cerebellar development and sensory and sensorimotor requirements of animals living disparate lifestyles.[15]

生理

编辑

Metabolism

编辑

Neurons are typically energy-intensive cells that have a high cost of maintenance, yet birds have high neuron densities and absolute numbers in their brains. Of the energy consumed by the brain, about 70-80% is used by neurons. The large numbers of neurons in the avian brain are enabled by relatively low specific energy demands. Experiments with pigeons have found the glucose demand of avian neurons to be more than 3 times less that of mammals, whose neuron energy costs do not change between taxa. It is not yet known why avian brains require so much less glucose, but two contributing factors, neuron size and body temperature, have been posited by researchers. Although there has not been definitive study on the topic as of 2023, it is speculated that bird neurons are smaller than those of mammals, since bird brains have a much higher density of neurons per unit volume compared to similarly sized mammals. Smaller neurons can consume less energy, as their smaller surface area and volume contain fewer receptors, ion channels, and mitochondria, while possessing lower membrane capacitance, meaning membrane potential requires less energy to change. A smaller neuron also requires less energy for upkeep. Additionally, the higher temperatures of bird brains, reaching 42 °C(108 °F) in pigeons, also facilitate lower energy consumption. Higher temperatures reduce the time it takes to activate and deactivate ion channels, which reduces energy needs.[16]

演化

编辑

The avian brain today differs markedly from those of mammals, from which the bird lineage diverged in the Permian, and from all other reptiles, from which birds diverged in the Triassic.

The nidopallium caudolaterale of the avian brain, responsible for goal-directed action, has been found in crocodylians, the closest living relatives of birds, separated evolutionarily by 245 million years. Though this could represent an example of convergent evolution between crocodylians and birds, scientists believe it is more likely that the last common ancestor of birds and crocodiles possessed a nidopallium caudolaterale.[17]

中生代

编辑

Birds evolved from the non-avian dinosaurs, and the dinosaurean lineage leading up to birds shows substantial enlargement and reshaping of the brain. Endocasts of dinosaur braincases allow scientists to examine the relationship between brain size and body size in extinct species. In particular, brain sizes underwent several significant enlargements from the first theropod dinosaurs to modern birds. The brain sizes and shapes of most dinosaurs are similar to those of modern reptiles.[18] However, in Coelurosauria, a group of dinosaurs containing (among other taxa) tyrannosauroids and birds, the brain is at least twice as large as other dinosaurs of similar body size.[19] A group of coelurosaurs, the Maniraptoriformes, a group containing birds as well as ornithomimosaurs, possesses brains again twice as large as those of other coelurosaurs.[19] Brain size increases again in the Maniraptora, a group containing birds and oviraptorosaurs.[19] Additionally, the shape of the brain acquired in Maniraptora, with a large cerebellum and optic lobe lateral to the telencephalon,[20] is found in Ichthyornis, a bird that lived 70 million years ago and closely related to modern birds.[21] The understanding of the evolution of brain size in modern birds is complicated by the limited sample of stem-bird brain endocasts. Only four Mesozoic avialans have brain endocasts: Archaeopteryx, which is not always classified as an avialan (closer to modern birds than to Dromaeosaurus),[19] Cerebavis, an incomplete specimen which lacks a hyperpallium and whose classification is unclear, Ichthyornis, and MPM-334-1, a basicranium that belonged to an eighty-million-year old enantiornithine bird, preserving the hindbrain.[21][22] No telencephalon is known from definitive examples of Enanitornithes, the largest and most diverse group of birds outside of the radiation of modern birds.[21]

 
Bird brain shapes from Deinonychus to modern crown birds

Archaeopteryx is traditionally considered the first bird, and is sometimes referred to as the Urvogel (original bird).[23] Despite possessing a brain almost certainly adapted for flight, the telencephalon of Archaeopteryx was not particularly large compared to related dinosaurs. Indeed, several oviraptorosaurs, as well as the troodontine Zanabazar and the jinfengopterygine troodontid[24] IGM100/1126, possess brains larger relative to their body size than Archaeopteryx. Other elements of the cranial anatomy of Archaeopteryx are likewise similar to those of other maniraptoran dinosaurs, suggesting that those animals may also have had the requisite neurological facilities for flight.[25]

The Wulst, the physically projecting hyperpallium, is of interest in bird brain evolution because it is not present in any other living reptiles other than birds. Moreover, it is thought to be analogous to the neocortex in mammals, with an important role in higher cognition. No non-avialan dinosaurs possess a Wulst, and indeed neither does Archaeopteryx, a primitive bird. However, the ornithuran Ichthyornis, despite having a brain shape resembling primitive maniraptorans, possesses a Wulst, showing that the structure likely originated earlier in bird evolution and exists outside of modern birds.[26]

In general, the brain-to-body ratio of dinosaurs doubled from basal Theropoda to Coelurosauria and again doubled from Coelurosauria to Maniraptoriformes. From Maniraptoriformes, the general form of the brain took upon a form that would be retained at least in Ichthyornis, close to modern birds.[27] Three major grade shifts in brain-to-body ratio are inferred by scientists to have taken place in the evolution of birds from basal Paraves to the base of crown Neoaves.[28]

新生代

编辑

There is substantial diversity in the size and arrangement of the brain in modern birds. During the Paleogene, the average brain size in birds relative to the body tended to increase; in many of the lineages emerging from the Paleogene avian radiation, brain size tended to scale more quickly than body size.[29][30] Scientists hypothesise that this growth in brain size occurred as a result of the adaptive radiation birds experienced at the K-Pg transition, and that the larger brains of crown birds allowed them to adapt to an unstable environment.[29]

The tremendous diversity of modern birds leads to a diverse range of patterns for the brain. In the clade Neoaves, comprising all birds save fowl and paleognaths, the brain-to-body ratio increases, but this is driven primarily by a decrease in average body size.[31][32] This pattern is observed in swifts, hummingbirds, sandpipers, buttonquails, as well as in the line leading up to Telluraves, the "higher landbirds". However, many groups of waterbirds, collectively known as the Aequornithes, do not follow this trend, rather tending to increase body size and brain size at an equal rate. The basalmost Telluraves are two branches of large, predatory birds. One, in Afroaves, is made up of the owls, which have large brains evolved for visual acuity, and the Accipitriformes, including hawks and eagles. The other, in Australaves, includes the falcons, seriemas, and the extinct terror birds. Within Afroaves, there are successive shifts towards higher brain-to-body ratios. The mousebirds and rollers in Coraciimorphae have greater brain-to-body ratios than Afroaves, and the woodpeckers nested within Coraciimorphae have yet larger ratios still.[32]

Some of the largest brain-to-body ratios in birds, especially of the telencephalon, that part of the brain responsible for cognition, are found in the Psittaciformes (parrots) and Corvidae (crows, ravens, jays, magpies, and allies), both members of the Australaves.[33] Indeed, the parrots and corvids are unique among birds for their large brain sizes. Moreover, scientists believe that their increased brain-body ratios evolved the most rapidly of any brain-body ratio shift in birds.[34]

In contrast to what is currently thought to be relatively few grade changes in the brain-to-body ratio of birds in the Mesozoic, researchers have found that nine such shifts took place in the Paleocene, possibly as a response to the K-Pg extinction event. Moreover, they find that the largest brain sizes in birds evolved only recently, with the Neogene radiation of crown corvids and crown parrots. Moreover, in these lineages, the density of neurons in the brain also increases, contributing to significant cognitive complexity.[35]

与哺乳动物的比较

编辑

The brains of birds are often compared to those of mammals. The earliest neuroscientists to extensively study birds, such as Ludwig Edinger, were struck by the differences they observed between bird brains and mammals. Today, the relationships between homologous structures in avian and mammalian brains are better known; additionally, many convergent features of bird and mammal brains have been observed. Some of the similarities between bird and mammal brains include the processing of specialised sensory input, involvement in higher cognition, high neuron density, and the fibre structure of the brain. Moreover, avian brains show evidence for sensory consciousness.[36] Many of the structures thought to contribute to mammal intelligence, such as the six-layered neocortex, are absent in birds. Despite this, birds such as corvids and parrots display intellectual behaviour that are comparable to those of highly intelligent mammals like the great apes. Scientists believe that this is an example of convergent evolution, wherein radically different gross structures evolved towards connectional similarities that produced comparable results.[37]

Striation of the pallium

编辑

In mammals, the neocortex, strongly associated with higher thought and cognition, is laminated and consists of 6 layers. In birds, there is no comparable lamination in most of the pallium, which corresponds to the cerebral cortex.[38] However, the hyperpallium of the dorsal telencephalon does indeed possess some layering: either three or four strata, depending on the size of the hyperpallium of the bird in question. In both the mammalian neocortex and the avian hyperpallium, the layers have distinct connection types and neurochemical composition.[39]

Analogues to the prefrontal cortex

编辑

The prefrontal cortex of mammals, which is highly involved in the processes that support complex learning, may have an analogue in birds in the form of the nidopallium caudolaterale. Researchers have found that there are similarities between the ways neurons in the caudolateral nidopallium activate in response to certain tests designed for the prefrontal cortex similarly to how neurons in the prefrontal cortex themselves behave. Damage to the caudolateral nidopallium also causes deficiencies in this class of task. However, the role of the caudolateral nidopallium is still not clear, and it has been tested only in pigeons, which possess relatively small brains. The role of the caudolateral nidopallium in more complex goal-oriented action is thus uncertain.[40][41]

历史

编辑

The scientific study of the avian brain accelerated in the 19th century as new techniques for the preparation of vertebrate brains became more refined. At the turn of the 20th century, the German neurologist Ludwig Edinger devised a theory of vertebrate cerebral evolution that soon became the dominant scientific view. The Dutch neurologist C.U. Ariëns-Kappers then refined the model, adapting it to the avian brain. Following a linear model of evolution, Ariëns-Kappers promoted the idea that brain structures were gained in sequence in evolutionary lineages, called an accretionary theory. According to his theory, birds and mammals inherited a primitive basal ganglia from fish and amphibians known as the palaeostriatum. They also inherited a newer basal ganglia from the reptiles known as the neostriatum. Reptiles were supposed to have evolved the neostriatum into two different structures, which were then passed on to birds. Ariëns-Kappers also posited that the topmost layer of the avian brain was formed of striatal brain cells, comprising a mass which he dubbed the hyperstriatum. Therefore, for many years, the avian telencephalon was thought to consists almost entirely of a large basal ganglia.[42] The first stereotaxic atlas of the avian brain, in 1967, followed the Ariëns-Kappers view of the avian brain and further entrenched the incorrect concept of a "primitive" avian brain.[43]

For years, too, scientists assumed that birds were not capable of advanced thought, as their brains were perceived to be devoid of complex pallial structures. Moreover, they lacked striated structures such as those found in the mammalian cerebral cortex, which were thought to be responsible for complex cognition. In fact, neurologists believed that birds were creatures that acted on instinct rather than on any sort of thought, and that they were highly unintelligent. From this conception arose the term colloquial term bird-brain, used to denigrate persons as unintelligent.[44]

Research into bird cognition, behaviour, and anatomy, as well as into the brain, specifically, it became apparent that the traditional accretionary view of vertebrate telencephalic evolution was incorrect. It was also becoming clear that what were then referred to as striatal parts of the brain were really pallial in origin. In the late 1990s and early 2000s, a movement began, culminating in 2002, resulting in a new nomenclature for the avian brain that has since become standard.[45] New research into bird neural networks resulted in a better understanding of the structure and organisation of the avian brain.[46]

参见

编辑

参考资料

编辑
  1. ^ Jarvis, Erich D.; Güntürkün, Onur; Bruce, Laura; Csillag, András; Karten, Harvey; Kuenzel, Wayne; Medina, Loreta; Paxinos, George; Perkel, David J.; Shimizu, Toru; Striedter, Georg. Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005-02-01, 6 (2): 151–159. ISSN 1471-0048. PMC 2507884 . PMID 15685220. doi:10.1038/nrn1606 (英语). 
  2. ^ Rose, Jonas. The avian brain (PDF). Current Biology Primers. 2022-10-24, 32 (20): R1076–R1079 [2023-10-08]. PMID 36283368. S2CID 253105801. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2022.07.072. (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2023-10-09). 
  3. ^ Güntürkün, Onur; von Eugen, Kaya; Packheiser, Julian; Pusch, Roland. Avian pallial circuits and cognition: A comparison to mammals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. Evolution of Brains and Computation. 2021-12-01, 71: 29–36 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0959-4388. PMID 34562800. S2CID 237603077. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2021.08.007 . (原始内容存档于2023-10-09). 
  4. ^ Rose, Jonas. The avian brain (PDF). Current Biology Primers. 2022-10-24, 32 (20): R1076–R1079 [2023-10-08]. PMID 36283368. S2CID 253105801. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2022.07.072. (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2023-10-09). 
  5. ^ 5.0 5.1 Rogers, Lesley J. Development and function of lateralization in the avian brain. Brain Research Bulletin. Special Issue:Brain Mechanisms, Cognition and Behaviour in Birds. 2008-06-15, 76 (3): 235–244. ISSN 0361-9230. PMID 18498936. S2CID 26031891. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.02.001. 
  6. ^ Moorman, Sanne; Gobes, Sharon M. H.; van de Kamp, Ferdinand C.; Zandbergen, Matthijs A.; Bolhuis, Johan J. Learning-related brain hemispheric dominance in sleeping songbirds. Scientific Reports. 2015-03-12, 5 (1): 9041. Bibcode:2015NatSR...5E9041M. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 4356971 . PMID 25761654. doi:10.1038/srep09041 (英语). 
  7. ^ Kuenzel, Wayne J.; Medina, Loreta; Csillag, Andras; Perkel, David J.; Reiner, Anton. The avian subpallium: New insights into structural and functional subdivisions occupying the lateral subpallial wall and their embryological origins. Brain Research. 2011-11-18, 1424: 67–101. ISSN 0006-8993. PMC 3378669 . PMID 22015350. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.037. 
  8. ^ Scanes, Colin G.; Dridi, Sami. Sturkie's Avian Physiology 7th. Academic Press. 2021: 257–274. ISBN 9780323853514 (英语). 
  9. ^ Zhang, Yutao; Zhou, Lifang; Zuo, Jiachun; Wang, Songhua; Meng, Wei. Analogies of human speech and bird song: From vocal learning behavior to its neural basis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2023, 14. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 9992734 . PMID 36910811. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1100969 . 
  10. ^ Scanes, Colin G.; Dridi, Sami. Sturkie's Avian Physiology 7th. Academic Press. 2021: 257–274. ISBN 9780323853514 (英语). 
  11. ^ Scanes, Colin G.; Dridi, Sami. Sturkie's Avian Physiology 7th. Academic Press. 2021: 257–274. ISBN 9780323853514 (英语). 
  12. ^ Scanes, Colin G.; Dridi, Sami. Sturkie's Avian Physiology 7th. Academic Press. 2021: 257–274. ISBN 9780323853514 (英语). 
  13. ^ Sultan, Fahad. Why some bird brains are larger than others. Current Biology. 2005-09-06, 15 (17): R649–R650 [2023-11-07]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 16139191. S2CID 6559127. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.043 . (原始内容存档于2024-02-24). 
  14. ^ Iwaniuk, Andrew N.; Hurd, Peter L.; Wylie, Douglas R.W. Comparative Morphology of the Avian Cerebellum: I. Degree of Foliation. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2006-06-14, 68 (1): 45–62 [2023-11-07]. ISSN 0006-8977. PMID 16717442. S2CID 1633943. doi:10.1159/000093530. (原始内容存档于2024-02-24). 
  15. ^ Iwaniuk, Andrew N.; Hurd, Peter L.; Wylie, Douglas R.W. Comparative Morphology of the Avian Cerebellum: II. Size of Folia. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2006-11-13, 69 (3): 196–219 [2023-11-07]. PMID 17108672. S2CID 2747025. doi:10.1159/000096987. (原始内容存档于2024-02-24). 
  16. ^ von Eugen, Kaya; Endepols, Heike; Drzezga, Alexander; Neumaier, Bernd; Güntürkün, Onur; Backes, Heiko; Ströckens, Felix. Avian neurons consume three times less glucose than mammalian neurons. Current Biology. 2022-10-10, 32 (19): 4306–4313.e4 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 36084646. S2CID 252124960. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2022.07.070 . (原始内容存档于2024-02-24). 
  17. ^ Güntürkün, Onur; von Eugen, Kaya; Packheiser, Julian; Pusch, Roland. Avian pallial circuits and cognition: A comparison to mammals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. Evolution of Brains and Computation. 2021-12-01, 71: 29–36 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0959-4388. PMID 34562800. S2CID 237603077. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2021.08.007 . (原始内容存档于2023-10-09). 
  18. ^ Larsson, Hans C. E.; Sereno, Paul C.; Wilson, Jeffrey A. Forebrain enlargement among nonavian theropod dinosaurs. Journal of Vertebrate Paleontology. 2000-09-25, 20 (3): 615–618 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0272-4634. S2CID 29413008. doi:10.1671/0272-4634(2000)020[0615:FEANTD]2.0.CO;2. (原始内容存档于2023-10-09) (英语). 
  19. ^ 19.0 19.1 19.2 19.3 Holtz, Thomas R. The Complete Dinosaur 2nd. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. 2012: 358–372. ISBN 9780253008497 (英语). 
  20. ^ Alonso, Patricio Domínguez; Milner, Angela C.; Ketcham, Richard A.; Cookson, M. John; Rowe, Timothy B. The avian nature of the brain and inner ear of Archaeopteryx. Nature. 2004-08-05, 430 (7000): 666–669 [2023-10-08]. Bibcode:2004Natur.430..666A. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 15295597. S2CID 4391019. doi:10.1038/nature02706. (原始内容存档于2022-12-05) (英语). 
  21. ^ 21.0 21.1 21.2 Torres, Christopher R.; Norell, Mark A.; Clarke, Julia A. Bird neurocranial and body mass evolution across the end-Cretaceous mass extinction: The avian brain shape left other dinosaurs behind. Science Advances. 2021-07-30, 7 (31). Bibcode:2021SciA....7.7099T. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 8324052 . PMID 34330706. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abg7099 (英语). 
  22. ^ Chiappe, Luis M.; Navalón, Guillermo; Martinelli, Agustín G.; Nava, William; Field, Daniel J. Fossil basicranium clarifies the origin of the avian central nervous system and inner ear. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2022-09-28, 289 (1983). ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 9515635 . PMID 36168759. doi:10.1098/rspb.2022.1398 (英语). 
  23. ^ The Urvogel's Old, New Clothes. Science. 2014-07-02 [2023-10-08]. (原始内容存档于May 16, 2021) (英语). 
  24. ^ Hartman, Scott; Mortimer, Mickey; Wahl, William R.; Lomax, Dean R.; Lippincott, Jessica; Lovelace, David M. A new paravian dinosaur from the Late Jurassic of North America supports a late acquisition of avian flight. PeerJ. 2019-07-10, 7: e7247. ISSN 2167-8359. PMC 6626525 . PMID 31333906. doi:10.7717/peerj.7247  (英语). 
  25. ^ Balanoff, Amy M.; Bever, Gabe S.; Rowe, Timothy B.; Norell, Mark A. Evolutionary origins of the avian brain. Nature. 2013-07-31, 501 (7465): 93–96 [2023-10-08]. Bibcode:2013Natur.501...93B. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 23903660. S2CID 4451895. doi:10.1038/nature12424. (原始内容存档于2023-10-31) (英语). 
  26. ^ Torres, Christopher R.; Norell, Mark A.; Clarke, Julia A. Bird neurocranial and body mass evolution across the end-Cretaceous mass extinction: The avian brain shape left other dinosaurs behind. Science Advances. 2021-07-30, 7 (31). Bibcode:2021SciA....7.7099T. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 8324052 . PMID 34330706. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abg7099 (英语). 
  27. ^ Holtz, Thomas R. The Complete Dinosaur 2nd. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. 2012: 358–372. ISBN 9780253008497 (英语). 
  28. ^ Ksepka, Daniel T.; Balanoff, Amy M.; Smith, N. Adam; Bever, Gabriel S.; Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Bourdon, Estelle; Braun, Edward L.; Burleigh, J. Gordon; Clarke, Julia A.; Colbert, Matthew W.; Corfield, Jeremy R. Tempo and Pattern of Avian Brain Size Evolution. Current Biology. 2020-06-08, 30 (11): 2026–2036.e3 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 32330422. S2CID 216095924. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.060 . hdl:11336/141993 . (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2020-07-16). 
  29. ^ 29.0 29.1 Bird Brain Evolution. American Scientist. 2021-09-23 [2023-10-08]. (原始内容存档于2022-07-07) (英语). 
  30. ^ Ksepka, Daniel T.; Balanoff, Amy M.; Smith, N. Adam; Bever, Gabriel S.; Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Bourdon, Estelle; Braun, Edward L.; Burleigh, J. Gordon; Clarke, Julia A.; Colbert, Matthew W.; Corfield, Jeremy R. Tempo and Pattern of Avian Brain Size Evolution. Current Biology. 2020-06-08, 30 (11): 2026–2036.e3 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 32330422. S2CID 216095924. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.060 . hdl:11336/141993 . (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2020-07-16). 
  31. ^ Bird Brain Evolution. American Scientist. 2021-09-23 [2023-10-08]. (原始内容存档于2022-07-07) (英语). 
  32. ^ 32.0 32.1 Ksepka, Daniel T.; Balanoff, Amy M.; Smith, N. Adam; Bever, Gabriel S.; Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Bourdon, Estelle; Braun, Edward L.; Burleigh, J. Gordon; Clarke, Julia A.; Colbert, Matthew W.; Corfield, Jeremy R. Tempo and Pattern of Avian Brain Size Evolution. Current Biology. 2020-06-08, 30 (11): 2026–2036.e3 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 32330422. S2CID 216095924. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.060 . hdl:11336/141993 . (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2020-07-16). 
  33. ^ Olkowicz, Seweryn; Kocourek, Martin; Lučan, Radek K.; Porteš, Michal; Fitch, W. Tecumseh; Herculano-Houzel, Suzana; Němec, Pavel. Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016-06-28, 113 (26): 7255–7260. Bibcode:2016PNAS..113.7255O. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 4932926 . PMID 27298365. doi:10.1073/pnas.1517131113  (英语). 
  34. ^ Ksepka, Daniel T.; Balanoff, Amy M.; Smith, N. Adam; Bever, Gabriel S.; Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Bourdon, Estelle; Braun, Edward L.; Burleigh, J. Gordon; Clarke, Julia A.; Colbert, Matthew W.; Corfield, Jeremy R. Tempo and Pattern of Avian Brain Size Evolution. Current Biology. 2020-06-08, 30 (11): 2026–2036.e3 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 32330422. S2CID 216095924. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.060 . hdl:11336/141993 . (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2020-07-16). 
  35. ^ Ksepka, Daniel T.; Balanoff, Amy M.; Smith, N. Adam; Bever, Gabriel S.; Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Bourdon, Estelle; Braun, Edward L.; Burleigh, J. Gordon; Clarke, Julia A.; Colbert, Matthew W.; Corfield, Jeremy R. Tempo and Pattern of Avian Brain Size Evolution. Current Biology. 2020-06-08, 30 (11): 2026–2036.e3 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 32330422. S2CID 216095924. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.060 . hdl:11336/141993 . (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2020-07-16). 
  36. ^ Ball, Gregory F.; Balthazart, Jacques. Evolutionary neuroscience: Are the brains of birds and mammals really so different?. Current Biology. 2021-07-12, 31 (13): R840–R842 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 34256914. S2CID 235802619. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2021.05.004 . (原始内容存档于2023-10-09). 
  37. ^ Emery, Nathan J.; Clayton, Nicola S. Evolution of the avian brain and intelligence (PDF). Current Biology Primers. 2005, 15 (23): R946–R950 [2023-10-08]. PMID 16332520. S2CID 8969952. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.029. (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2022-07-26). 
  38. ^ Ball, Gregory F.; Balthazart, Jacques. Evolutionary neuroscience: Are the brains of birds and mammals really so different?. Current Biology. 2021-07-12, 31 (13): R840–R842 [2023-10-08]. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 34256914. S2CID 235802619. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2021.05.004 . (原始内容存档于2023-10-09). 
  39. ^ Emery, Nathan J.; Clayton, Nicola S. Evolution of the avian brain and intelligence (PDF). Current Biology Primers. 2005, 15 (23): R946–R950 [2023-10-08]. PMID 16332520. S2CID 8969952. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.029. (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2022-07-26). 
  40. ^ Emery, Nathan J.; Clayton, Nicola S. Evolution of the avian brain and intelligence (PDF). Current Biology Primers. 2005, 15 (23): R946–R950 [2023-10-08]. PMID 16332520. S2CID 8969952. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.029. (原始内容存档 (PDF)于2022-07-26). 
  41. ^ Liu, Xinyu; Wan, Hong; Li, Shan; Shang, Zhigang; Shi, Li. The role of nidopallium caudolaterale in the goal-directed behavior of pigeons. Behavioural Brain Research. 2017-05-30, 326: 112–120. ISSN 0166-4328. PMID 28288807. S2CID 4282299. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2017.02.042. 
  42. ^ Emery, Nathan J. Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005-12-07, 361 (1465): 23–43. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 1626540 . PMID 16553307. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1736 (英语). 
  43. ^ Reiner, Anton; Perkel, David J.; Mello, Claudio V.; Jarvis, Erich D. Songbirds and the Revised Avian Brain Nomenclature. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004-06-16, 1016 (1): 77–108. Bibcode:2004NYASA1016...77R. ISSN 0077-8923. PMC 2481519 . PMID 15313771. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.013 (英语). 
  44. ^ Emery, Nathan J. Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005-12-07, 361 (1465): 23–43. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 1626540 . PMID 16553307. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1736 (英语). 
  45. ^ Reiner, Anton; Perkel, David J.; Mello, Claudio V.; Jarvis, Erich D. Songbirds and the Revised Avian Brain Nomenclature. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004-06-16, 1016 (1): 77–108. Bibcode:2004NYASA1016...77R. ISSN 0077-8923. PMC 2481519 . PMID 15313771. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.013 (英语). 
  46. ^ Harding, Cheryl F. Learning from bird brains: how the study of songbird brains revolutionized neuroscience. Lab Animal. 2004-05-01, 33 (5): 28–33 [2023-10-09]. ISSN 0093-7355. PMID 15141244. S2CID 751995. doi:10.1038/laban0504-28. (原始内容存档于2023-10-09).